Fwd: Did language drive society or vice versa?

From: Wade T.Smith (wade_smith@harvard.edu)
Date: Tue May 09 2000 - 14:45:12 BST

  • Next message: Chuck Palson: "Re: Central questions of memetics"

    Received: by alpheratz.cpm.aca.mmu.ac.uk id OAA14932 (8.6.9/5.3[ref pg@gmsl.co.uk] for cpm.aca.mmu.ac.uk from fmb-majordomo@mmu.ac.uk); Tue, 9 May 2000 14:48:42 +0100
    Subject: Fwd: Did language drive society or vice versa?
    Date: Tue, 9 May 2000 09:45:12 -0400
    x-sender: wsmith1@camail2.harvard.edu
    x-mailer: Claris Emailer 2.0v3, Claritas est veritas
    From: "Wade T.Smith" <wade_smith@harvard.edu>
    To: "memetics list" <memetics@mmu.ac.uk>
    Content-Type: text/plain; charset="iso-8859-1"
    Content-transfer-encoding: quoted-printable
    Message-ID: <20000509134547.AAA21399@camailp.harvard.edu@[128.103.125.215]>
    Sender: fmb-majordomo@mmu.ac.uk
    Precedence: bulk
    Reply-To: memetics@mmu.ac.uk
    

    SCIENCE MUSINGS Did language drive society or vice versa?

    http://www.boston.com/dailyglobe2/130/science/Did_language_drive_society_or
    _vice_versa_+.shtml

    By Chet Raymo, 5/9/2000

    Here's a sentence from a report on the evolution of language in a recent
    issue of Nature:

    ''A challenge for evolutionary biology, therefore, is to provide a
    detailed mathematical account of how natural selection can enable the
    emergence of human language from animal communication.''

    A lovely, complex sentence of the kind we used to diagram in high school.

    I doubt if kids diagram sentences any more, but I loved diagramming. If
    nothing else, it gave us a sense of how a manageable number of
    grammatical rules (syntax) could give rise to an endless variety of
    communications.

    What a thing is language. Start with a bunch of noises - vowels and
    consonants; three or four dozen will do nicely. String them together into
    words and you have enough combinations to have a verbal expression for
    millions of different people, places, things and actions. A babe is born
    into the world knowing nary a word. By age 2, she will have a few hundred
    words at her command. An adult might have a working vocabulary of tens of
    thousands of words.

    But even then, we don't go around grunting single words. We put words
    together into meaningful sentences using the rules of syntax. And
    suddenly the number of possible utterances becomes essentially infinite.
    ''Hop on Pop'' is a possibility. So is the Holy Bible or ''Finnegans
    Wake.''

    Where did it all come from? When and where did language evolve? Chimps
    and gray parrots can be taught to communicate in a pared-down version of
    human language, but the difference between human speech and the most
    sophisticated natural animal communication is as different as day and
    night.

    No animal communication except our own is syntactic, or so it seems. The
    territorial calls of birds, the wiggle dance of bees, and the mysterious
    vocalizations of whales and dolphins are the best we get in non-human
    nature. Yet the language of even the most ''primitive'' human culture is
    as complex as modern English. Clearly, language took a big leap forward
    as the human brain exploded in size and complexity. Like all humans on
    the planet, all spoken tongues can be traced back to a common source.

    The fact that all human languages have grammatical similarities suggests,
    as Chomsky observed, an innate correspondence between language and the
    brain. But whether the acquisition of language drove development of the
    brain, or a bigger, more complex brain was a prerequisite for syntactic
    language is a question no one can yet answer.

    Can language be explained in evolutionary terms? That's the question
    asked by Martin Nowak, Joshua Plotkin and Vincent Jansen in the Nature
    article. Their method is mathematical but their conclusion is simple:
    Natural selection will favor the development of syntactic communication
    when the number of ''relevant communication topics'' surpasses a certain
    minimum number.

    By ''relevant communication topics'' the researchers mean anything in a
    speaker's environment that confers a survival advantage if you can talk
    about it. ''The lion is lurking in the grass'' might be such a message,
    and it is easy to see how accurate communication of this message would
    give the speaker and his listener an edge.

    But what made our hominid ancestors' environment more complex - more
    ''relevant communication topics'' - than that of jackals, dolphins or
    gray parrots? Surely other species experience just as many threats to
    survival, maybe more. Our authors say: ''Presumably the increase in the
    number of relevant communication topics was caused by changes in the
    social structure.'' Which seems to say exactly nothing by way of
    explanation. We are right back to the chicken and the egg: Did language
    drive social structure or was it the other way around?

    Nowak, Plotkin and Jansen show how syntactic language might have evolved,
    but we are left with the mystery of why it evolved (presumably) uniquely
    for the human species. Something truly wonderful happened in the East
    African grasslands a few million years ago - the appearance of big,
    complex brains articulating a sophisticated repertoire of combinatorial
    sounds - and it seems to have happened all of a piece.

    It is easy enough to understand the evolutionary pressures that caused
    our ancestors to say ''The lion is lurking in the grass'' or ''There's a
    nice source of flinty stones just beyond the hill,'' but where, for
    heaven's sake, did ''The sunset is beautiful'' come from? And what
    dynamic of natural selection conferred upon one species among all the
    others the ability to say:

    ''She walks in beauty, like the night
    Of cloudless climes and starry skies,
    And all that's best of dark and bright
    Meet in her aspect and her eyes?''

    Chet Raymo is a professor of physics at Stonehill College and the author
    of several books on science.

    This story ran on page E02 of the Boston Globe on 5/9/2000. © Copyright
    2000 Globe Newspaper Company.

    ===============================This was distributed via the memetics list associated with the
    Journal of Memetics - Evolutionary Models of Information Transmission
    For information about the journal and the list (e.g. unsubscribing)
    see: http://www.cpm.mmu.ac.uk/jom-emit



    This archive was generated by hypermail 2b29 : Tue May 09 2000 - 14:49:02 BST