Fwd: Thinking Like a Chimp

From: Wade T.Smith (wade_smith@harvard.edu)
Date: Thu Nov 16 2000 - 14:26:03 GMT

  • Next message: Mark Mills: "Re: Fwd: Thinking Like a Chimp"

    Received: by alpheratz.cpm.aca.mmu.ac.uk id OAA17604 (8.6.9/5.3[ref pg@gmsl.co.uk] for cpm.aca.mmu.ac.uk from fmb-majordomo@mmu.ac.uk); Thu, 16 Nov 2000 14:29:28 GMT
    Subject: Fwd: Thinking Like a Chimp
    Date: Thu, 16 Nov 2000 09:26:03 -0500
    x-sender: wsmith1@camail2.harvard.edu
    x-mailer: Claris Emailer 2.0v3, Claritas Est Veritas
    From: "Wade T.Smith" <wade_smith@harvard.edu>
    To: "memetics list" <memetics@mmu.ac.uk>
    Content-Type: text/plain; charset="US-ASCII"
    Message-ID: <20001116142444.AAA5410@camailp.harvard.edu@[128.103.125.215]>
    Sender: fmb-majordomo@mmu.ac.uk
    Precedence: bulk
    Reply-To: memetics@mmu.ac.uk
    

    Thinking Like a Chimp

    by Rabiya S. Tuma
    Posted November 10, 2000 Issue 90
    http://news.bmn.com/hmsbeagle/90/notes/feature2

    Abstract

    Researchers have long used the behavior of nonhuman animals to find clues
    about human cognition. Daniel J. Povinelli's studies suggest that humans
    may be unique in their possession of a mental awareness. His work also
    highlights the difficulties and controversies that arise when studying
    how an animal thinks.

    So your friend talks to his car like it is a sentient being? Don't be
    alarmed. According to Daniel J. Povinelli at the University of
    Louisiana's New Iberia Research Center, you are just witnessing a
    uniquely human result of evolution. After spending the last decade
    studying chimpanzee cognition, Povinelli is convinced that humans and
    their closest living relatives have very different minds. "Humans
    constantly invoke unobservable phenomena and variables to explain why
    certain things are happening," says Povinelli. "Chimps operate in the
    world of concrete, tangible things that can be seen. The content of their
    minds is about the observable world."

    The desire to understand animal cognition, particularly that of animals
    resembling ourselves, has a long tradition behind it. Charles Darwin
    concluded that if an animal's behavior is nearly identical to ours, then
    the mental activity supporting that behavior will also be very similar.

    But just because an animal behaves like us, does that really mean that
    similar mental activity drives their behavior? Povinelli isn't so sure.
    But then he is something of an exception in a field that has long
    emphasized the similarities shared by chimps and humans, and has given
    little airtime to the differences.

    Certainly there is no doubt that chimps are intelligent animals that
    interact flexibly with their environment. But how similar are the minds
    of chimps and humans?

    To get at the answer, comparative psychologists and anthropologists
    design simple experiments, often set up like games, to test whether
    chimpanzees are aware of their own thoughts and those of others. After
    all, awareness of thought is a hallmark of human cognition.

    In one such experiment, Povinelli's group tested whether or not the
    chimps understood the concept of seeing. Chimps regularly use information
    gathered through sight, but the question is, Do they think about seeing
    or know about others' experiences of seeing?

    The experiment is relatively simple and relies on chimpanzees' natural
    begging gesture. When a chimp wants something, it stretches out its hand,
    palm up, to the person or chimp with the desired object, be it a piece of
    food or a toy. In the experiment, the chimp enters a room and is
    confronted with two experimenters with food. One experimenter has a
    blindfold over his eyes and the other one doesn't; obviously, the
    experimenter with his eyes covered can't see the chimp enter the room or
    its request for the food. Povinelli reasoned that if the chimps
    understood the concept of seeing, then they would only ask for the food
    from the experimenter who could see them. But in numerous trials, and in
    variations on this theme, the chimpanzees did not discriminate between
    the two experimenters, begging equally often from the experimenter who
    could not see them.

    Povinelli's interpretation of these data is that chimps don't think about
    seeing or about what others see. Other researchers aren't so sure. For
    example, Josep Call at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary
    Anthropology in Leipzig, Germany, thinks the data could be interpreted
    differently. "If you go to the street and you ask people, 'What is the
    part of your eye that allows you to see? Is it the retina? Is it the
    eyeball? Is it the iris?', some people," he points out, "will not know
    what part of the eye allows you to see. That does not mean that they do
    not know anything about seeing." Similarly, he thinks Povinelli's
    blindfold experiment tests more about whether a chimp knows which part of
    the anatomy is necessary for seeing than about whether the animal thinks
    about seeing.

    This difference in interpretation highlights what is so intractable about
    the field of chimpanzee cognition: How do you ask how a nonverbal animal
    thinks when it can't tell you? Testing what it thinks about is relatively
    easy, but is it aware of its own mental states?

    In all known cultures, Povinelli points out, humans constantly evaluate
    their interactions with others in terms of unobservable emotions, desires
    and beliefs, asking questions such as "What is she thinking? Why did he
    do that? Was he angry? Or did she believe such and such was true?" to
    explain the observable actions.

    But Povinelli has yet to find evidence that chimps use this sort of
    abstract thinking about unobservable things, and he thinks that this sort
    of mental awareness is uniquely human.

    "I think, based on a lot of empirical research that we have done," says
    Povinelli, "that humans and chimps share a common set of low-level
    psychological operations that drive most of our behavior. But during the
    course of human evolution, we wove in alongside those ancient neural and
    psychological systems - in parallel to them - an additional, more
    abstract and conscious way of representing those behaviors and thinking
    about those behaviors. That more abstract way of thinking about the world
    appeals to such unobservable things as beliefs and desires, intentions,
    goals, and internal unobservable emotions to explain why the social world
    is behaving the way it is, why people behave the way they do.

    "I think it is intimately tied up with the evolution of our language
    capacities," concludes Povinelli. "The representational capacities that
    underwrite language can support this kind of abstract reasoning
    capability."

    If Povinelli is right that this sort of abstract thinking about the world
    around us is the instinctual result of evolution, then trying to discern
    how another type of mind, such as the chimp's, conceptualizes the world
    forces researchers to challenge their own evolution. In order to discover
    and describe how another type of mind works - one that doesn't use
    abstractions - the researcher has to check the involuntary attribution of
    emotions and logic to the chimp's actions.

    "It is very difficult to shut down our human way of thinking about the
    world and step into the mind of another species," says Povinelli. Yet if
    researchers want to understand how another species thinks, then they must
    find another way to conceptualize thought.

    The instinctual attribution of emotion to other animals, and even to
    inanimate objects like your friend's car, might also explain why
    cognition researchers have focused so much on the similarities between
    chimpanzees and humans. It is, after all, easier to assume that what is
    happening in our minds is what is happening in the mind of an animal that
    looks and behaves so much like us.

    But Povinelli emphasizes that, in the end, all of the reproducible data
    about chimp cognition needs to be accounted for within a single
    theoretical framework. And although he stands out in his view that humans
    are uniquely cognizant of their emotions and thoughts, he is convinced
    that this is the only theory that will accommodate all of the data. After
    all, he concludes, "In every critical juncture, after the chimp has
    learned something and we gave him the option to tell us, 'are you really
    reasoning about seeing or are you using some surface behavior cue?', at
    every case, they have consistently said, 'What are you talking about? We
    are using what is there. We're using what is in the world.'"

    Rabiya S. Tuma is a freelance science writer based in Oregon and New York.

    ===============================================================
    This was distributed via the memetics list associated with the
    Journal of Memetics - Evolutionary Models of Information Transmission
    For information about the journal and the list (e.g. unsubscribing)
    see: http://www.cpm.mmu.ac.uk/jom-emit



    This archive was generated by hypermail 2b29 : Thu Nov 16 2000 - 14:31:02 GMT