Fwd: From The Undiscovered Mind:

From: Wade T.Smith (wade_smith@harvard.edu)
Date: Sun Feb 06 2000 - 04:09:55 GMT

  • Next message: Ilfryn PRICE: "Recognition at last?"

    Received: by alpheratz.cpm.aca.mmu.ac.uk id EAA12181 (8.6.9/5.3[ref pg@gmsl.co.uk] for cpm.aca.mmu.ac.uk from fmb-majordomo@mmu.ac.uk); Sun, 6 Feb 2000 04:11:29 GMT
    Subject: Fwd: From The Undiscovered Mind:
    Date: Sat, 5 Feb 2000 23:09:55 -0500
    x-sender: wsmith1@camail2.harvard.edu
    x-mailer: Claris Emailer 2.0v3, Claritas Est Veritas
    From: "Wade T.Smith" <wade_smith@harvard.edu>
    To: "Memetics Discussion List" <memetics@mmu.ac.uk>, "SKEPTIC-L" <skeptic@listproc.hcf.jhu.edu>
    Content-Type: text/plain; charset="iso-8859-1"
    Content-transfer-encoding: quoted-printable
    Message-ID: <20000206041528.AAA4544@camail2.harvard.edu@[205.240.180.154]>
    Sender: fmb-majordomo@mmu.ac.uk
    Precedence: bulk
    Reply-To: memetics@mmu.ac.uk
    

    The Undiscovered Mind
    http://www.biomednet.com/hmsbeagle/71/xcursion/essay

    From The Undiscovered Mind: How the Human Brain Defies Replication,
    Medication, and Explanation (pp. 28-32)

    by John Horgan

    © 1999 by John Horgan. Used with permission of The Free Press. Posted
    February 4, 2000 · Issue 71

    Editor's note: At the behest of the Society for Neuroscience, the 1990's
    were designated by the U.S. Congress as the "Decade of the Brain." In The
    Undiscovered Mind, John Horgan describes the reaction of neuroscientist
    and Nobel laureate Torsten Wiesel to the concept of this timeframe - it
    was "foolish." "We need at least a century, maybe even a millennium, to
    comprehend the brain." And that could be a conservative estimate -
    indeed, as Horgan notes, the science of the mind may never have an end.

    Horgan explores the many facets of mind-science, including neuroscience,
    Freudian analysis, behavioral genetics, evolutionary psychology, the
    philosophy of consciousness, artificial intelligence, and more. He
    describes the remarkable research being done in these fields, and talks
    with the researchers themselves about their sometimes astonishing
    results. But ultimately, Horgan finds in all this research what
    philosophers call an "explanatory gap" - while many mental apparatus can
    be elegantly described, the ultimate "how" of the mind remains as elusive
    as ever. And then, as we see below, there's the small matter of emotions.

    Getting in Touch with Emotions

    Even if they unravel the mechanisms underlying working memory and other
    cognitive functions, neuroscientists must face another problem: How does
    emotion fit into the puzzle? Until recently many neuroscientists sought
    to sidestep emotion in their experiments, treating it as an annoying
    source of experimental noise and distortion rather than a fundamental
    part of human nature. Neuroscientists have followed the lead of cognitive
    scientists, who have tried to understand those information-processing
    functions that can be most easily duplicated in computers, such as
    vision, recollection, speech recognition, and reasoning.

    By avoiding emotion, neuroscientists and cognitive scientists have
    created a peculiarly one-dimensional picture of the mind, according to
    Joseph LeDoux, a neuroscientist at New York University. Cognitive science
    "is really a science of only a part of the mind, the part having to do
    with thinking, reasoning, and intellect," LeDoux complained in his 1996
    book, The Emotional Brain. "It leaves emotions out. And minds without
    emotions are not really minds at all. They are souls on ice - cold,
    lifeless creatures devoid of any desires, fears, sorrows, pains, or
    pleasures."

    LeDoux, himself a cool, controlled man with deep-set eyes and a carefully
    trimmed beard, has demonstrated that at least one emotion, fear, can be
    approached empirically. Unlike language or other cognitive functions
    unique to humans, LeDoux pointed out, fear is a biological phenomenon
    whose roots reach back far into the history of life. The neural circuitry
    and processes that underlie fear have been highly conserved through
    evolution; thus experiments on rats and other animals may reveal much
    about humans. The amygdala, which is crucial to the fear response, is
    found not only in humans and primates but also in rats.

    "The fear system is very, very simple," LeDoux told me. "You've got a
    stimulus that comes in through standard input channels, goes to the
    amygdala and goes out through the output channels," he said. Early
    studies of fear responses had produced confusing results because the
    experiments were too complex. "Every time you change the experiment, you
    change the way the brain accomplishes the task. So the key in figuring
    out the fear system is to strip it down to a simpler model."

    LeDoux has carried out experiments in which rats have been conditioned to
    associate a certain sound, such as a musical tone, with an unpleasant
    sensation, such as an electric shock. The initial response of rats and
    many other animals to such a stimulus is to freeze, an appropriate tactic
    for an animal threatened by a predator. The freeze response is an innate,
    reflexive function. LeDoux and his colleagues showed that damage to a
    minute structure within the amygdala, called the lateral nucleus,
    prevented rats from learning to freeze in response to the tone preceding
    an electric shock. The cognitive ability of the rats was unimpaired in
    other respects.

    LeDoux was trying to unravel the circuitry required for more complex
    fear-related behavior, which is sometimes called instrumental learning.
    For example, when a rat learns that freezing does not prevent him from
    being shocked, he tries avoidance - moving to a different part of the
    cage or climbing up its sides. At this point, the rat makes the
    transition from being an emotional reactor to an actor, LeDoux said,
    capable of making choices and trying different strategies.

    Psychologists once believed that the subjective sensation of fear is the
    first component of the fear response; increased heart rate, sweating, and
    other physiological symptoms were thought to be triggered by the
    subjective sensation. LeDoux contended that the opposite is probably
    true; physiological symptoms occur first and then initiate the subjective
    sensation of fear. In many cases, moreover, the fear response might never
    generate a conscious sensation. Our conscious, subjective feelings "are
    red herrings, detours, in the scientific study of emotions," LeDoux has
    written.

    LeDoux felt that too much attention had been paid to consciousness
    lately. "It would surely get you the Nobel Prize if you figured it out,"
    he told me, "but I don't think it would tell us what we need to know"
    about the mind. Although consciousness is often equated with the mind,
    most mental processes occur beneath the level of awareness, LeDoux
    pointed out. Consciousness, moreover, is a relatively recent innovation
    of evolution. "Basically the brain is unconscious. Somewhere in evolution
    consciousness evolved as a module. It's connected up to some other parts
    of the brain, but not the rest of it."

    Explaining consciousness is not as important as understanding how the
    brain draws on both genes and experience to create a self, a personal
    identity, in each individual. "That to me is the big question: how our
    brain makes us who we are. Explaining consciousness wouldn't explain
    that." The key to this issue is understanding how both nature and nurture
    affect the brain's wiring. "What's often overlooked is that nature and
    nurture speak the same language, which is the synaptic language," LeDoux
    said. Ultimately all influences on personality, genetic or experiential,
    become manifest at the level of the connections between neurons.

    LeDoux doubted whether any single theory would account for emotion. There
    are many aspects of emotion, he noted. "There's an evolutionary
    component, there's a cognitive component, a behavioral component. It's
    just a question of what the balance in the particular situation is."
    Cognitive theories tend to focus on conscious emotional processes;
    evolutionary theories emphasize innate emotional responses; behavioral
    theories stress the role of environmental conditioning. "In any
    particular emotional episode, it's not a matter of which one is right but
    which one explains which part of the episode." Moreover, each emotion
    probably requires a separate explanation; the mechanisms underlying fear
    are probably quite different from those underlying lust or hatred.

    LeDoux summarized the research that he and others have done on emotion,
    and particularly fear, in The Emotional Brain. He also cautiously
    suggested that investigations of the neurobiology of fear might at some
    point yield better treatments for human anxiety disorders. LeDoux
    expected psychiatrists to dismiss his rat experiments as irrelevant to
    their work. But to his surprise, psychiatrists responded to his book
    enthusiastically - almost too enthusiastically, LeDoux suggested. "It's
    been almost this uncritical acceptance," he explained. "'Yes, let's go!
    This is the answer!' They seem so desperate. I don't think I have the
    answers in my book. I just threw out some ideas."

    Like Gerald Fischbach, Torsten Wiesel, and other leading neuroscientists,
    LeDoux readily acknowledges the shortcomings of his field. He once
    stated, "We have no idea how our brains make us who we are. There is as
    yet no neuroscience of personality. We have little understanding of how
    art and history are experienced by the brain. The meltdown of mental life
    in psychosis is still a mystery. In short, we have yet to come up with a
    theory that can pull all this together. We haven't yet had a Darwin,
    Einstein or Newton."

    Then LeDoux suggested that neuroscience might not need a unifying theory:

    Maybe what we need most are lots of little theories. It would be great to
    know how anxiety or depression works, even if we don't have a theory of
    mental illness. And wouldn't it be wonderful to know how we experience a
    wonderful piece of music (be it rock or Bach), even in the absence of a
    theory of perception. And to understand fear or love in the absence of a
    theory of emotion in general wouldn't be so bad either. The field of
    neuroscience is in a position to make progress on these problems, even if
    it doesn't come up with a theory of mind and brain.

    John Horgan is a freelance writer and author of The End of Science, a
    U.S. best-seller that has been translated into ten languages. His awards
    include the Science Journalism Award of the American Association for the
    Advancement of Science (1992 and 1994) and the National Association of
    Science Writers Science-in-Society Award (1993).

    ©2000 BioMedNet Ltd. All rights reserved.

    ===============================This was distributed via the memetics list associated with the
    Journal of Memetics - Evolutionary Models of Information Transmission
    For information about the journal and the list (e.g. unsubscribing)
    see: http://www.cpm.mmu.ac.uk/jom-emit



    This archive was generated by hypermail 2b29 : Sun Feb 06 2000 - 04:11:32 GMT